Effects of Sport on Symptoms of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder and Performance Loss Caused by Distractors: Evaluation with MOXO d-CPT
PDF
Cite
Share
Request
Original Article
P: 137-145
July 2023

Effects of Sport on Symptoms of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder and Performance Loss Caused by Distractors: Evaluation with MOXO d-CPT

Turk J Child Adolesc Ment Health 2023;30(2):137-145
1. Hatay Training and Research Hospital, Clinic of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Hatay, Turkey
2. Amasya University Faculty of Medicine, Department of Public Health, Amasya, Turkey
3. Marmara University Faculty of Medicine, Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Istanbul, Turkey
4. Nigde Provincial Health Directorate, Nigde, Turkey
5. Kırsehir Provincial Health Directorate, Kırsehir, Turkey
No information available.
No information available
Received Date: 02.11.2021
Accepted Date: 16.02.2022
Publish Date: 01.08.2023
PDF
Cite
Share
Request

ABSTRACT

Objectives:

Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is the most common childhood psychiatric disorder characterized by inattention, hyperactivity, and impulsivity. Treatment modalities should include medication, psychotherapy, and psychosocial treatments. Various studies concluded that sport has a positive effect on attention. The present study evaluates if football significantly improve the possible psychopathology of ADHD and reduce the negative impact of distractors.

Materials and Methods:

One hundred forty-seven children from a sport school in a small city in Turkey were included in the current descriptive study. The MOXO distracted-CPT (d-CPT) (distracted-computerized continuous performance test) was applied to all children. Socio-demographic data form, behavior evaluation inventory for executive functions, and Conner’s parent rating scale were applied to the parents.

Results:

According to the MOXO d-CPT results, there was no significant difference between the group with possible psychopathology and the group with no possible psychopathology in terms of sport duration. But, there was a negative correlation between sport duration and attention Z score, timing Z score, and impulsivity Z score in the group that included children who showed poor performance or had possible psychopathology at the MOXO d-CPT. Further, the median value of sport duration was 6.03 months in the group with possible psychopathology severity 4, and the median value was 8.06 months in the group with possible psychopathology severity 1-2-3 at MOXO d-CPT. That is, the possible psychopathology severity decreased as sport duration increased. Also, there was a moderately negative correlation between sport duration and the score lost with visual distractor and/or auditory distractor in the MOXO d-CPT.

Conclusion:

In conclusion, these findings revealed that exercise in football, a group game, improves or decreases the possible psychopathology severity and reduces the negative impact of visual and/or auditory distractors on MOXO d-CPT parameters.

Introduction

Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is the most common childhood psychiatric disorder; It is a neurodevelopmental disorder characterized by inattention, hyperactivity, and impulsivity.1-3 ADHD is a clinical diagnosis, and there is no specific test for diagnosis. Interviews with family and children, clinical observation, psychiatric examination, scales completed by parents and teachers are among the tools that assist the clinician in making a diagnosis.4 Due to these difficulties in diagnosis and the subjective nature of measurement tools, neuropsychological tests are used to diagnose. Recently, computerized continuous performance tests (CPTs) have been frequently among neuropsychological tests.5 The basic paradigm in CPTs involves selectively paying attention to or being alert to the non-continuous stimulus.6 Distracted-CPT (d-CPT) also contains distracting stimuli to mimic everyday environments.7

Executive function impairments were detected in neuropsychological tests performed on children with ADHD.8 A meta-analysis includes eighty-three studies; It has been reported that there is significant impairment in all functions of executive functions in ADHD, especially; in response inhibition (arrest of motor response), vigilance, working memory, and planning.9 Executive dysfunction seen in ADHD has negative effects in various areas of daily life.3 Solving the behavioral, cognitive, social, and familial problems that characterize ADHD is the first goal of treatment.4 ADHD is often treated pharmacologically using methylphenidate. Because of its proven efficacy and high response rate often the first choice of current treatment modalities.10-12 Treatment modalities should include medication, psychotherapy, and psychosocial treatments. Considering that 10.0 % to 30.0 % of children with ADHD give an insufficient clinical response to drug treatment, adjuvant therapies are required.13

For this reason, the management of cognitive impairment, which has adverse effects in many areas of ADHD, is not only limited to drug therapy, the benefits of physical exercise have also been accepted in the literatüre.14,15 It has been reported that physical exercise has positive effects on physical,16 behavioral,17 cognitive,18,19 psychosocial health,20 sleep.21,22 It has been reported that physical activity performed for ten weeks, 45 min 3 times a week for children with ADHD improves attention (continuous and split).23 Similarly, another study found that six weeks of athletic activity improved the attention, cognitive symptoms, and social skills of children with ADHD.24 Some studies found that dopamine is released in the prefrontal cortex and basal ganglia during sports activity25 and that adrenaline, noradrenaline concentrations increase during physical activity.26 The biological hypothesis of ADHD is based on catecholamine dysfunction such as adrenaline, noradrenaline, or dopamine.27,28 According to the results of all these studies, it can be concluded that sport has a positive effect on attention.

Based on the literature above-mentioned, physical exercise increases catecholamine release and positively affects attention. We evaluated the children enrolled in a football school with the MOXO d-CPT, assuming that football will significantly improve the possible psychopathology of ADHD and reduce the negative impact of distractors.

Material and Method

Participants and Procedures

One hundred forty-seven children enrolled in a sport school in Niğde were included in the current study. MOXO d-CPT was applied to all children. The school enrollment dates of eight of the children who underwent MOXO d-CPT, and five are missing scale data. During the MOXO d-CPT application, along with the Socio-demographic data form, behavior evaluation inventory for executive functions (BRIEF), Conner’s parent rating scale (CPRS) were applied to the parents. The sport duration is calculated in months by subtracting the child’s enrollment date to the sport school from the application date of MOXO d-CPT. According to the curriculum information obtained from the sport school, it was determined that the children did football exercises for 2 h once a week.

This investigation was approved by the Ethics Committee of Niğde Ömer Halisdemir University (decision no: 2019/33, date: 04.10.219). The children and their families received information about the purposes and methods of the study. Written and verbal consent were obtained from all children and parents.

Survey Instrument

MOXO d-CPT ADHD test; It is an objective measurement tool designed to detect ADHD-related symptoms, applied online via a computer, with 90.0% sensitivity and 85.0% specificity. It measures the four components of ADHD, attention, timing, hyperactivity, and impulsivity, separately. As in other continuous performance tests, the participant’s task in this test is to maintain his attention on stimuli that are in a continuous flow and react (response) by pressing the space key on the keyboard only once, as soon as he sees the predetermined target image. The most important feature that distinguishes MOXO from other continuous performance tests is that it contains measurable distractors. In MOXO, the participant’s attention, timing, impulsivity, and hyperactivity are evaluated under distractors by simulating real-life stimuli. It is possible to assess the variability of all these parameters with distractors one by one. There are eight parts in MOXO. There are no distractors in the first and last parts. There are single and double visual distractors in two parts, and there are single and double auditory distractors in the other two. In the last two parts, there is a single and double visual and auditory distractor. After the test is completed, the system automatically presents the personal performance chart and attention profile. The performance chart allows to measure the person’s performance over time during the test and evaluate the distractors’ effects. Test profile gives a Z score for each parameter according to the age and gender of the person. The performance is classified according to the Z score, such as;

Z score is ≥0: good performance

-0.825≤ Z score <0: average and above

-1.65≤ Z score <-0.825: poor performance

Z score is <-1.65: out-of-normal distribution

If the Z score is out-of-normal distribution, the program presents a severity rating of 1-2-3-4 (1= low violence, 4= extreme violence) according to the Z score.29,30

The group of out of the normal distribution was defined as “possible psychopathology” in the current study. Score loss with distractors was obtained from the database of the d-CPT implementing company.

BRIEF; is a scale that evaluates the behaviors of children and adolescents (5-18 years old) in different environments within the framework of executive functions.31 Turkish reliability and validity studies have been done.32 There is a parent form and a teacher form. There are 86 expressions reflecting the way of behaving in both forms. Items in the BRIEF are organized under eight sub-scales. Two indicator scores are calculated as the behavioral regulation index (BRI) and the metacognition index (MI). The global manager score (GMS) is calculated from the two indicator scores (DBRI and MI). BRI consists of three sub-scales: Emotional control, set changing, and suppression, while MI consists of five sub-scales: planning/organizing, working memory, initiation, regularity, and monitoring. The items in the scale are evaluated on a 3-point scale as; never (1), sometimes (2), and often (3). A high score in BRIEF indicates impairment in executive functions.

CPRS; The original form was developed in 1978 to evaluate children’s knowledge about hyperactivity, learning, and behavioral problems through their parents’ information.33 The scale includes five subscales: behavioral problem, aggression /hyperactivity, learning problem, anxiety and psychosomatic problems, and 48 items. Items in the scale are evaluated on a 4-point Likert-type scale, and the options of “never”, “rarely”, “often” and “always” in the scale are; It is scored as “0”, “1”, “2” and “3”. The higher the total score obtained from the scale and the higher the subscales’ scores indicate a high level of behavioral problems.

Statistical Analysis

Statistical analyses were performed using the SPSS statistical package version 17 (Chicago, IL: SPSS Inc.). The Shapiro-Wilk test was used to evaluate the compliance with a normal distribution. The Mann-Whitney U test was used for non-normally distributed variables. The Spearman Correlation test was used to assess the correlations. Values were expressed as numbers, frequencies, percentages, mean and standard deviation, or median and minimum-maximum values, and p values <0.05 were considered statistically significant (two-tailed).

Results

The mean age of the children included in the study was found to be 9.8±1.7years (n=142). All participants were male. The median value of the time children was enrolled in sport school was 7.1 (0.9-44.6) months (n=139). In the study, 70 of 147 (47.6%) children who underwent MOXO d-CPT had possible psychopathology in at least one of the parameters of attention-timing-impulsivity-mobility and, 82 of them (55.8%) either had possible psychopathology or showed poor performance in at least one of these four parameters. It was observed that the possible psychopathology severity was 3 or 4 in 55 (78.6%) of 70 children with possible psychopathology according to MOXO d-CPT. According to the MOXO d-CPT results, there was no significant difference between the group with possible psychopathology and the group with no possible psychopathology in sport duration (p=0.083). According to the MOXO d-CPT results, the median value of sport duration was 6.0 (1.0-24.3) months in the group with possible psychopathology severity 4, and the median value was 8.1 (0.9-12.2) months in the group with possible psychopathology severity 1-2-3 (p=0.025) (Table 1).

Table 1

A two-stage analysis was performed in the correlation analysis between the sport duration and the Z score of the MOXO d-CPT parameters. In the first stage, 67 children outside the normal distribution in at least one parameter according to the MOXO d-CPT results were included in the analysis. According to the result of this analysis, a low level of negative correlation between the timing Z score and the sport duration (r=-0.275); A low-level positive correlation (r=0.258) was found between the impulsivity Z score and the sport duration (p<0.05). In the second stage, 78 children who showed poor performance or had possible psychopathology in at least one parameter according to the MOXO d-CPT results were included in the analysis. According to the results of this analysis, a low level of negative correlation between the sport duration and attention Z score (r=-0.240), a moderate negative correlation between the timing Z score (r=-0.301); A low-level positive correlation (r=0.258) was found between the impulsivity Z score and the sport duration (p<0.05) (Table 2).

Table 2

A two-stage analysis was used to analyze the correlation between the sport duration and the loss of score observed with distractors in the MOXO d-CPT parameters. In the first stage, 67 children outside the normal distribution in at least one parameter according to the MOXO d-CPT results were included in the analysis. Among these children, those who lost points for the respective distractors were included in the analysis. A moderately negative correlation was found between sport duration and the score lost with one visual distractor (r=-0.590), two auditory distractors (r=-0.534), and one combination distractor (r=-0.343) in the MOXO d-CPT attention parameter. A moderate negative correlation (r=-0.494) was found between sport duration and the score lost with one auditory distractor in the hyperactivity parameter (p<0.05). In the second stage, 78 children who showed poor performance or had possible psychopathology in at least one parameter according to MOXO d-CPT results were included in the analysis. Among these children, those who lost points for the respective distractors were included in the analysis. A moderate negative correlation was found between the sport duration and the loss of score in the attention parameter (r=-0.534) and the loss of score in the hyperactivity parameter (r=-0.419) between two auditory distractors in the MOXO d-CPT. A low-level negative correlation (r=-0.283) was found between the two visual distractors and the loss of score in the timing parameter. Also, a moderate positive correlation (r=0.380) was found between an auditory distractor and loss of score in the impulsivity parameter (p<0.05) (Table 3).

Table 3

Among the scores of BRIEF subscale; between initiation, planning, monitoring subscale scores, behavioral regulation index, MI, GMS, and MOXO d-CPT attention sub-parameter Z score; between all subscale scores except “organization of materials” subscale, behavioral regulation index, MI, GMS and MOXO d-CPT timing parameter Z score; There was a low-level positive correlation between the baseline subscale score and the Z score of the MOXO d-CPT impulsivity parameter (p < 0.05). No correlation was found between the sub-scale scores of BRIEF, behavioral regulation index, metacognitive index, and GMS, and the MOXO d-CPT hyperactivity sub-parameter Z score (Table 4).

Table 4

In the correlation analysis results between CPRS subscale scores and total scale score and Z scores of the four parameters of MOXO d-CPT; the score between the timing Z score and the Conner’s behavioral problem subscale (r=0.186) and the total scale (r=0.191) score There was a low level of positive correlation (p<0.05). There was no correlation with attention, impulsivity, and hyperactivity Z scores. No significant correlation was found between CPRS subscale scores and total score and sport duration.

Discussion

In the current study, we evaluated the children enrolled in a football school with regular sport, using the MOXO d-CPT, and evaluated whether the sport duration had a positive effect on attention, and we aimed to examine the effect of sport duration on visual and auditory distractors in children with possible psychopathology at least one of the parameters of attention-mobility-timing-impulsivity. In the literature, studies have shown that physical exercise and sport induce dopamine releases.25 Physical activity is a potent stimulator of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal and noradrenergic systems.34 Considering that the leading biological hypothesis of ADHD is based on catecholamine dysfunction;27,28 The question arises to how this effect of sport will affect ADHD symptoms.

In the current study, there was a negative correlation between sport duration and attention Z score, timing Z score, and impulsivity Z score in the group that included children with possible psychopathology or poor performance at the MOXO d-CPT. Studies show that regular physical exercise or sport has positive effects on cognitive functions and behavior control.35-40 A study conducted with children with poor impulse control and attention disorders has shown that physical exercise is associated with decreased negative behavior and improvements in cognitive functions.37

In the current study, when we classified the children who had possible psychopathology from one of the MOXO d-CPT’s parameters, according to the possible psychopathology severity; we found that the sport duration for those with low intensity (1 and 2) was longer than that of those with high intensity (3 and 4). This suggests that even if the sport duration does not eliminate the possible psychopathology, does it reduces the possible psychopathology severity? Studies indicate a positive relationship between the amount of physical exercise and academic performance in the literatüre.40-45 Similarly, in another study, it was stated that children with the higher aerobic condition had better inhibition.46 It has been reported in previous studies that a high aerobic fitness level is associated with attention-working memory-reaction time parameters and provides better performance in response accuracy.47 These findings are very interesting for children with ADHD when viewed considering Barkley’s theoretical model48, suggesting that inhibition is a fundamental deficiency of ADHD. Therefore, if physical exercise can improve inhibition and executive functions, it can provide an improvement in one’s self-regulation. As a result, it has been shown that physical activity or fitness level has beneficial effects on cognitive performance.46

We examined how the negative effects of auditory and visual distractors, which we cannot always disable in daily life, change with sport in children with possible psychopathology at least one of the parameters of the MOXO d-CPT. In particular, we found that the negative effect of visual and auditory distractors on attention decreased with the sport. As the duration of the sport increased, the loss of performance caused by distractors decreased. When the negative effect of distractors on attention was evaluated in detail, we assessed that in the section where one visual distractor is given, in the section where two auditory distractors are given, and in the combined section where one visual and one auditory distractor is given, the sport significantly decreases the performance loss caused by the distractor and has a healing effect. But all of these were related to the sport duration, so we thought that when children were allowed to exercise longer or more intensely, the negative impact on performance could be reduced with two visual distractors or in the combined part with two visual and two auditory distractors. A study in which three groups of children who did not exercise did little exercise and exercised more intensely; only improvement was observed in executive functions of children who exercised intensely.49 This suggests that the positive effects can only occur with a greater amount of physical activity. When the literature is reviewed, it has been reported that there are gains in cognitive functions in studies using aerobically-based intense exercise interventions.49-51

In the current study, performance loss with two auditory distractors decreased in the group that included children with possible psychopathology or poor performance at the MOXO d-CPT attention parameter. This means that even if the child does not have possible psychopathology; if the child shows poor performance in terms of attention, the negative effect caused by the auditory distractor decreases with the sport. In the same group, in the section where there were two visual distractors in the MOXO d-CPT, it was observed that the negative effect on timing decreased with the sport duration. When we evaluated the negative effects of distractors on hyperactivity; we found that an auditory distractor had a healing effect in the group that included children with possible psychopathology or poor performance. When we looked at the effect of distractors on impulsivity, we found a positive correlation: those who exercised longer were more impulsive. This may be because children with high impulsivity can be sent to sport more consistently by their families. These data, which we obtained using an objective measurement using the MOXO d-CPT, disable subjective evaluations with the healing anticipation effect of sport. In this respect, we think that it will make a significant contribution to the literature. Also, since there is no study evaluating the negative effect of distractors in the literature, this study is important in providing novel information.

We found a positive correlation between the Z scores of the attention and timing parameters of the MOXO d-CPT and the BRIEF behavioral regulation index, MI, and GMS. Since ADHD, which is characterized by behavioral control, attention, and reasoning deficit, is known to have executive dysfunctions,52,53 it is not surprising that the MOXO d-CPT parameters predict the possible psychopathology of ADHD, and the BRIEF scale scores are correlated with Z scores of MOXO d-CPT.

Finally, it is necessary to highlight a few issues whose answers are not yet known. Considering whether the type of exercise affects the results or not, interventions in physical education, after fitness, or aerobic exercise, all have positive effects, and the type of exercise is unimportant.38 So, how is the result affected in group sports? The coordination required for group activity and a more strategic approach to group games (e.g., planning) raises whether it could positively affect executive functions. For this reason, the answer to the question “Does the exercise of children alone or in a group change the effect on cognitive functions, is one more effective than the other?” It can be evaluated in future studies. Besides, referring to a study that states that physical activity is at the highest level in children of secondary and primary school age,38 it is beneficial to conduct more comprehensive future studies in a wide age range to understand how sports vary by age group. Another important issue with an unknown answer is “Whether the improvements in cognition caused by exercise continue or decrease after the end of physical activity”. So, studies in a prospective design are needed after the sport is quit.

Study Limitations

One of the strengths of the current study is that it is the first study to evaluate how visual and auditory distractors, which have negative effects on possible ADHD symptoms, affect performance loss change with the sport. Latter; By objectively measuring possible psychopathology with MOXO d-CPT, we have eliminated the bias that will occur with subjective measurement. Because the anticipation or the Halo effect can affect the positive response between physical activity and cognition.39 The first of the limitations of the current study is; It has a retrospective pattern. In this prospectively planned study, we had to retrospectively change the design of the study, as the football school was closed due to the coronavirus disease-2019 pandemic. Latter; The fact that the study was conducted with a relatively small sample limits the statistical power of the study. Third; The sport practiced in the football school is done once a week. More positive results can be obtained using more frequent and intensive exercise. Therefore, the results of this study should be regarded as a preliminary assessment and repeated. However, it can be useful to guide future research. Finally, this study analyzed how long the child has attended sport school. Another limitation of the study is that physical activities outside the sport school were not evaluated.

Conclusion

In conclusion, these findings revealed that exercise in football, a group game, decreases possible psychopathology severity and negative impact of visual and/or auditory distractors on the performance. Our findings seem promised for additional research is showing the positive effect of sport, especially on attention problems in children with ADHD. Physical exercise can be used as an additional supportive treatment to increase the effectiveness of pharmacological treatment in children with ADHD or as an alternative non-pharmacological treatment in children who cannot be given pharmacological treatment.

Acknowledgments

The financial resource required for MOXO d-CPT was obtained by researchers. There is no conflict of interest in the current study.

Ethics

Ethics Committee Approval: This investigation was approved by the Ethics Committee of Niğde Ömer Halisdemir University (decision no: 2019/ 33, date: 04.10.219).

Informed Consent: Written and verbal consent were obtained from all children and parents.

Peer-review: Internally peer-reviewed.

Authorship Contributions

Concept: M.E., E.D., E.Değ., Design: M.E., N.Y., E.D., E.Değ., Data Collection or Processing: M.E., N.Y., E.D., E.Değ., Analysis or Interpretation: M.E., N.Y., Ü.G., Literature Search: M.E., N.Y., Ü.G., Writing: M.E., N.Y., Ü.G., E.D., E.Değ.

Conflict of Interest: No conflict of interest was declared by the authors.

Financial Disclosure: The authors declared that this study received no financial support.

References

1
American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-5. In: Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 2013.
2
Faraone SV, Biederman J. Neurobiology of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 1998;44:951-958.
3
Barkley RA. Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions: constructing a unifying theory of ADHD. Psychol Bull. 1997;121:65-94.
4
Kayaalp L. Dikkat eksikliği hiperaktivite bozukluğu. Sürekli Tıp Eğitimi Etkinlikleri Sempozyum Dizisi. 2008;62:147-152.
5
Berger I, Cassuto H. The effect of environmental distractors incorporation into a CPT on sustained attention and ADHD diagnosis among adolescents. J Neurosci Methods. 2014;222:62-68.
6
Riccio CA, Reynolds CR, Lowe P, Moore JJ. The continuous performance test: a window on the neural substrates for attention? Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2002;17:235-272.
7
Berger I, Slobodin O, Aboud M, Melamed J, Cassuto H. Maturational delay in ADHD: evidence from CPT. Front Hum Neurosci. 2013;7:691.
8
Seidman LJ, Biederman J, Monuteaux MC, Valera E, Doyle AE, Faraone SV. Impact of gender and age on executive functioning: do girls and boys with and without attention deficit hyperactivity disorder differ neuropsychologically in preteen and teenage years? Dev Neuropsychol. 2005;27:79-105.
9
Willcutt EG, Doyle AE, Nigg JT, Faraone SV, Pennington BF. Validity of the executive function theory of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a meta-analytic review. Biol Psychiatry. 2005;57:1336-1346.
10
Bedard AC, Martinussen R, Ickowicz A, Tannock R. Methylphenidate improves visual-spatial memory in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2004;43:260-268.
11
Holmes J, Gathercole SE, Place M, Dunning DL, Hilton KA, Elliott JG. Working memory deficits can be overcome: Impacts of training and medication on working memory in children with ADHD.Applied cognitive psychology. 2010;24:827-836.
12
Klingberg T, Fernell E, Olesen PJ, Johnson M, Gustafsson P, Dahlström K, Gillberg CG, Forssberg H, Westerberg H. Computerized training of working memory in children with ADHD--a randomized, controlled trial. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2005;44:177-186.
13
Wigal SB. Efficacy and safety limitations of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder pharmacotherapy in children and adults. CNS Drugs. 2009;23:21-31.
14
Puyjarinet F, Franc N, Purper-Ouakil D. Pédopsychiatrie et psychomotricité: apports spécifiques, complémentarité thérapeutique et réflexions communes autour de la prise en charge des enfants TDA/H et de leur famille. Entretiens de Psychomotricité 2012. 2012:13-25.
15
McMorris TE, Tomporowski PE, Audiffren ME. Exercise and cognitive function. Wiley-Blackwell; 2009.
16
Organization WH. World health statistics 2010. World Health Organization; 2010.
17
Sowa M, Meulenbroek R. Effects of physical exercise on autism spectrum disorders: a meta-analysis, Research in autism spectrum disorders. 2012;6:46-57.
18
Kramer AF, Erickson KI. Capitalizing on cortical plasticity: influence of physical activity on cognition and brain function. Trends Cogn Sci. 2007;11:342-348.
19
Pontifex MB, Saliba BJ, Raine LB, Picchietti DL, Hillman CH. Exercise improves behavioral, neurocognitive, and scholastic performance in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Pediatr. 2013;162:543-551.
20
Netz Y, Wu MJ, Becker BJ, Tenenbaum G. Physical activity and psychological well-being in advanced age: a meta-analysis of intervention studies. Psychol Aging. 2005;20:272-284.
21
Youngstedt SD, O’Connor PJ, Dishman RK. The effects of acute exercise on sleep: a quantitative synthesis. Sleep. 1997;20:203-214.
22
Kubitz KA, Landers DM, Petruzzello SJ, Han M. The effects of acute and chronic exercise on sleep. A meta-analytic review. Sports Med. 1996;21:277-291.
23
Verret C, Guay MC, Berthiaume C, Gardiner P, Béliveau L. A physical activity program improves behavior and cognitive functions in children with ADHD: an exploratory study. J Atten Disord. 2012;16:71-80.
24
Kang KD, Choi JW, Kang SG, Han DH. Sports therapy for attention, cognitions and sociality. Int J Sports Med. 2011;32:953-959.
25
Wigal SB, Nemet D, Swanson JM, Regino R, Trampush J, Ziegler MG, Cooper DM. Catecholamine response to exercise in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder.Pediatr Res. 2003;53:756-761.
26
Zouhal H, Jacob C, Delamarche P, Gratas-Delamarche A. Catecholamines and the effects of exercise, training and gender. Sports Med. 2008;38:401-423.
27
Pliszka SR, McCracken JT, Maas JW. Catecholamines in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: current perspectives. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1996;35:264-272.
28
Zametkin AJ, Rapoport JL. Neurobiology of attention deficit disorder with hyperactivity: where have we come in 50 years? J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1987;26:676-686.
29
Cassuto H, Ben-Simon A, Berger I. Using environmental distractors in the diagnosis of ADHD. Front Hum Neurosci. 2013;7:805.
30
Solutions N. MOXO ADHD analytics Professional guide (Version EU 1.00). In: Israel; 2014.
31
Gioia GA, Isquith PK, Guy SC, Kenworthy L. Behavior rating inventory of executive function: BRIEF. Psychological Assessment Resources Odessa, FL; 2000.
32
Koylu SN. Turkish Translation, Reliability and Validity Studies of Behavior Rating Inventory Of Executive Function, (BRIEF). 2011;10:894-904.
33
Goyette CH, Conners CK, Ulrich RF. Normative data on revised Conners Parent and Teacher Rating Scales. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 1978;6:221-236.
34
Wittert G. The effect of exercise on the hypothalamo—pituitary—adrenal axis. In: Sports endocrinology. Springer; 2000:43-55.
35
Hillman CH, Erickson KI, Kramer AF. Be smart, exercise your heart: exercise effects on brain and cognition. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2008;9:58-65.
36
Tomporowski PD, Davis CL, Miller PH, Naglieri JA. Exercise and Children’s Intelligence, Cognition, and Academic Achievement. Educ Psychol Rev. 2008;20:111-131.
37
Tomporowski PD, Tinsley V. Effects of target probability and memory demands on the vigilance of adults with and without mental retardation. Am J Ment Retard. 1994;98:688-703.
38
Sibley BA, Etnier JL. The relationship between physical activity and cognition in children: a meta-analysis. Pediatric exercise science. 2003;15:243-256.
39
Shephard RJ. Curricular physical activity and academic performance. Pediatric exercise science. 1997;9:113-126.
40
Coe DP, Pivarnik JM, Womack CJ, Reeves MJ, Malina RM. Effect of physical education and activity levels on academic achievement in children. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2006;38:1515-1519.
41
Castelli DM, Hillman CH, Buck SM, Erwin HE. Physical fitness and academic achievement in third- and fifth-grade students. J Sport Exerc Psychol. 2007;29:239-52.
42
Caterino MC, Polak ED. Effects of two types of activity on the performance of second-, third-, and fourth-grade students on a test of concentration. Percept Mot Skills. 1999;89:245-248.
43
Keays JJ, Allison KR. The effects of regular moderate to vigorous physical activity on student outcomes: a review. Can J Public Health. 1995;86:62-65.
44
Shephard R, Lavallee H, Volle M, LaBarre R, Beaucage C. Academic skills and required physical education: The Trois Rivieres experience.CAHPER Research Supplement. 1994;1:1-12.
45
Shephard RJ. Habitual physical activity and academic performance. Nutr Rev. 1996;54:S32-S36.
46
Buck SM, Hillman CH, Castelli DM. The relation of aerobic fitness to stroop task performance in preadolescent children. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2008;40:166-172.
47
Hillman CH, Castelli DM, Buck SM. Aerobic fitness and neurocognitive function in healthy preadolescent children. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2005;37:1967-1974.
48
Barkley RA. ADHD and the nature of self-control. Guilford press; 1997.
49
Davis CL, Tomporowski PD, Boyle CA, Waller JL, Miller PH, Naglieri JA, Gregoski M. Effects of aerobic exercise on overweight children’s cognitive functioning: a randomized controlled trial. Res Q Exerc Sport. 2007;78:510-519.
50
Hinkle JS, Tuckman BW, Sampson JP. The psychology, physiology, and creativity of middle school aerobic exercisers. Elementary School Guidance & Counseling. 1993;28:133-145.
51
Tuckman BW, Hinkle JS. An experimental study of the physical and psychological effects of aerobic exercise on schoolchildren.Health Psychol. 1986;5:197-207.
52
Lyon G. The need for conceptual and theoretical clarity in the study of attention, memory, and executive function. Attention, memory, and executive function. 1996:3-9.
53
Naglieri JA. Current advances in assessment and intervention for children with learning disabilities. In: Advances in learning and behavioral disabilities. Emerald Group Publishing Limited; 2003.